Skip to main content

A model of cognitive evaluation battery for diagnosis of mild cognitive impairment and dementia in educated and illiterate Egyptian elderly people

Abstract

Background

The high illiteracy rates in the North African and Middle Eastern region make direct cognitive testing challenging. Validated instruments for dementia in Arabic language are lacking specially those targeting low-educated subjects.

Objectives

The aim of this study was to develop a cognitive evaluation battery suitable for both educated and illiterate Egyptian elderly people.

Design

A cross-sectional study was conducted. Setting: Ain-Shams University geriatric and ophthalmology wards, geriatrics outpatient clinic, and geriatric clubs. Participants: 159 male and female participants aged ≥ 60 years were recruited.

Measurements

Cut-off points were determined according to DSM-IV criteria for dementia and MMSE scores which divided the participants into 3 quadrants as normal, having mild cognitive impairment and having dementia then application of the new battery test was done.

Results

Test re-test reliability ranged from adequate to high in most of its tests with r ≥ 0.7. There was a statistical significance between all battery tests when divided into normal and dementia according to DSM IV criteria except in digit span forward length, digit span backward length, stimulus cue of confrontation naming and judgment. Means and standard deviations were calculated for each battery subset, for the whole sample, for low-educated group and group with > 9 years education according to three quadrants of MMSE.

Conclusion

A new valid and reliable neurocognitive evaluation battery that can differentiate between normal, mild cognitive impairment, and dementia in both educated and illiterate subjects under the name of Ain Shams Cognitive Assessment (ASCA) scale is now available.

Introduction

The word cognition comes from the Latin verb cognosco (con “with” and gnōscō “know”), which means to conceptualize or to recognize [1].

Cognitive decline is a normal process of aging [2]. Some cognitive abilities, such as vocabulary, may improve with age, while others such as memory and speed processing, decline gradually over time [3]. However, there is a significant heterogeneity among older adults in the rate of decline in those abilities [4].

With aging of the population, dementia became a public health problem increasing health care costs. It affects about 5–10% of elderly population above the age of 65 with doubling prevalence each 5 years till reaching about 50% at the age of 85 [5, 6]. The world Alzheimer’s report in 2018 stated that every 3 s, there is a new case of dementia [7] which needs our attention regarding this devastating problem.

In the USA, the prevalence of dementia from all causes was between 5 and 7% of adults above age of 60 years [8]. Crude prevalence rates of dementia in European Union countries in subjects over 65 years varied between 5.9 and 9.4% [9]. Regarding Asian countries, the most recent national survey conducted in Japan stated the prevalence of dementia in people aged ≥ 65 years to be 15.75% [10], while in Korea it was 8.7% [11].

In North Africa and the Middle East, studies about dementia prevalence are scarce. In a pilot study in Lebanon, dementia prevalence was 7.4% [12]. In Egypt, there is insufficient data about dementia prevalence all over the country; however, a systematic review including 6 studies from Assiut, New Valley, Red Sea, and Qena governorates reported that the prevalence was between 2.01 and 5.07% [13].

Literacy is strongly reflected in the performance of subjects in neuropsychological evaluations. Education influence visual perception, logical reasoning, learning, and recall strategies. The process of learning to read and write may train specific additional abilities, such as explicit phonological awareness and spatial perception. The inability of an illiterate subject to perform cognitive tasks, present in current neuropsychological test batteries, does not necessarily mean abnormal brain function. Even though people with lower educational levels have a higher rate of developing dementia, cognitive function cannot be accurately assessed due to the limitations of current available tools [14].

Tests that have already been standardized on illiterates in different countries are the Mini Mental State Examination (MMSE), the abbreviated neuropsychologic battery (NEUROPSI), Attention and Memory, verbal fluency tests, visual and spatial tests, executive function tests, and calculation and number processing. However, additional research is needed to determine their sensitivity to brain impairment and their prediction of functional abilities [15]. In 2015, Julayanont and colleagues developed the Montreal Cognitive Assessment-Basic (MoCA-B) to be the first assessment to screen for mild cognitive impairment (MCI) in illiterate elderly adults and those with low levels of education [16].

There is a significant diversity in literacy rates among countries and according to the Central Agency for Public Mobilization and Statistics (CAPMAS), Egypt has 18.4 million illiterate people with 6.9% of youths and 63.4% of the elderly are illiterate [17]. The high illiteracy rates in the North African and Middle Eastern region make direct cognitive testing challenging. Validated screening instruments for dementia in Arabic language are lacking [18].

Neuropsychological assessment is a performance-based method to evaluate cognitive functioning. Typically, it is performed with a battery approach, which involves tests of different cognitive ability areas such as memory, attention, processing speed, spatial, reasoning, judgment, language, and problem-solving functions. Neuropsychological assessment provides general and specific information about current levels of cognitive performance [19].

Inhabitants of the Arab world are 370 million, in addition to eight million Arab immigrants living outside their country of origin [20]. Egypt’s population alone makes up 23.6% of the Arabic-speaking population, and although Arabic is the fourth most commonly spoken language in the world [21], the availability of standardized neuropsychological tests in Arabic is scarce. This gives rise to the need of developing valid neuropsychological tests for these populations [22].

MMSE, MOCA, Wechsler memory scale, and Addenbrooke’s cognitive examination are available in Arabic; however, MMSE does not assess executive function and other tests are not suitable for illiterates.

Aim of the study

The aim of this study was to develop a cognitive evaluation battery for the diagnosis of MCI and dementia and the battery should be applicable for illiterates, low educated, and highly educated Egyptian elderly people and so, several neuropsychological batteries were reviewed including the National Institutes of Health Executive Abilities: Measures and Instruments for Neurobehavioral Evaluation and Research (NIH EXAMINER) [23], MOCA [24], and The Arabic version of Addenbrooke’s cognitive examination (ACE III) [22] before developing this battery.

Subjects and methods

Study design

This is a cross-sectional study.

Study population

One hundred and seventy community dwelling elderly participants were recruited during the period between May 2017 and July 2018 from relatives of patients in the geriatric and ophthalmology wards, patients attending geriatrics outpatient clinic, and geriatric clubs.

Inclusion criteria

Males and females aged 60 years or more and written consent was taken from all participants.

Exclusion criteria

Severe dementia, depression, people who had severe visual or hearing impairment, and functional impairment limiting participation in the study.

Participants were asked about their age, handedness, educational level, and family history of cognitive impairment, and then the following steps were done:

Step I: clinical diagnosis

  1. 1.

    National Institute of Neurological and Communicative Disorders and Stroke-Alzheimer’s Disease and Related Disorders Association (NINCDS-ADRDA) Core Clinical Criteria for dementia diagnosis [25]: These criteria imply that for dementia diagnosis, cognitive impairment needs to be confirmed by neuropsychological testing (bedside mental state examination or neuropsychological testing). Two or more of the following cognitive domains must be impaired for diagnosis: memory and learning, executive functions, language, visuospatial abilities, personality, and behavior, which significantly affect function.

  2. 2.

    Peterson’s clinical criteria for diagnosis of MCI [26] as follows:

    1) Memory complaint, preferably corroborated by an informant

    (2) Objective memory impairment for age

    (3) Relatively preserved general cognition for age

    (4) Essentially intact activities of daily living, and

    (5) Not demented

Step II: neuropsychological testing

I) Mini Mental State Examination (MMSE) [27]

  • This 28 point-scale was administered evaluating orientation, registration, attention and calculation, recall, language (repetition and complex command), and visuospatial functions.

  • For illiterate and low-educated subjects, attention was measured by reverse days of the week instead of serial calculations. Reading, writing, and drawing were scored as zero if the subject could not perform them.

II) DSM IV criteria for dementia

  • According to the American Psychiatric Association Diagnostic and Statistical Manual of Mental Disorders IV (DSM-IV) [28], participants were asked about the following:

  1. 1.

    Memory impairment plus at least one of the following:

  2. 2.

    Aphasia, apraxia, agnosia, or disturbance in executive functioning.

    • Participant were considered positive for dementia if the above domains were positive, affecting them socially or functionally and after the exclusion of having delirium at the present time.

After application of clinical and DSM IV criteria, participants were classified statistically into 3 quadrants according to results of MMSE after using the cutoff points for diagnosing dementia in Egyptian population defined previously [29]. Dementia was diagnosed if MMSE score ≤ 22 for participants > 9 years education and ≤ 21 for illiterate participants and those with < 9 years education. Clinical criteria were correlated with statistical classification before application of the battery.

III) New battery tests which include the following:

  1. 1.

    Verbal paired associated test

  1. (a)

    Verbal learning (VL)

Paired-associate learning test from Wechsler memory scale was used to measure the ability to form new links between two items and retrieval [30].

Following the test from original Wechsler [31, 32], a list of six related and four unrelated pairs was read followed by presentation of the first word in each pair; the participants provided the paired word and the number of correctly recalled pairs were scored over three learning trials.

  1. (b)

    Distractor interval

Following the previous learning trials, the participants were asked to count from 100 backwards, stopped after 28 s, and then asked about the previous pairs as before; the first word in each pair was introduced and the participants were asked to provide the paired word. The correctly recalled words were scored.

  1. (c)

    Delayed recall

  • Ten minutes following the 28-s recall, participants were given the first word in each pair and were asked to provide the paired word. The correctly recalled words were scored.

  • The 10 min were filled by non-verbal tests as not to act like intrusions in the recall [33].

  1. (d)

    Word recognition test

  • Participants were given a set of words and were asked to recognize if they were present in the paired associate or not.

  • Nine words were chosen from the paired associate and inserted among other 18 words from the same category to act as a novel prototype

  1. 2.

    Bender Gestalt (BG) copy and memory

  • Participants were asked to copy a figure to measure visual-motor skills [34] and then to copy it from their memory 10 min later to measure visual memory.

  1. 3.

    Digit forward and digit backward span length

  • Digit forward: to test short-term memory, participants were asked to repeat the number in the order told [35] and the correct span length recalled was scored.

  • Digit backwards: to test working memory, participants were asked to repeat the numbers told in a reverse order [36] and the correct span length was scored.

  • In both digits forward and digit backward, participants were given a second trial with the same span length but different numbers in each step if they failed to recall the correct order in the first time.

  1. 4.

    Set shifting

Trail making test B is a test to evaluate the part of executive functioning considered with the ability to quickly switch attention while inhibiting automatic responses [37]. The test consists of both numbers and letters, and participants are asked to connect numbers and letters alternatively [38], to make the test more suitable for illiterates a new trail making test was used with changes to suit illiterates under the name of Ain Shams Set Shifting for Illiterates (ASTI).

  1. 5.

    Letter/Lexical fluency

According to the Egyptian–Arabic ACE III [22], participants were asked to name items that start with the letter (sh); validated in ACE III in 60 s and 1 point was given for each correct answer.

  1. 6.

    Verbal fluency (spherical)

Participants were asked to name as many spherical shapes as possible in 60 s. One point was given for each correct item.

  1. 7.

    Verbal fluency test/category

According to the Egyptian–Arabic ACE III [22], participants were asked to name as many animals as possible in 60 s and 1 point was given for each correct item.

  1. 8.

    Language object naming test

  • Participants were asked to name 12 pictures with scoring of 0–12.

  • The test was adapted from Egyptian–Arabic ACE III [22].

  • Stimulus cues (describing what the item is about) if the participant misperceives the item.

  • Phonemic cuing (saying the phonetic sound of the word beginning) was given if the participant could not recognize the picture even after stimulus cue or if he/she seems to recognize it but cannot put the right name to it.

  • The correct items (identified without cuing) out of 12 was scored once and then total correct items (correct+ stimulus cue) out of 12 were scored.

  1. 9.

    Abstraction

  • Participants were asked about popular Egyptian metaphors.

  • Answers were scored according to the well-known base in Egyptian culture, e.g., in “Cut your coat according to your cloth” if the participant explained that someone should do as well as possible with the limited money they have is considered right. The metaphors were:

القرش الابيض ينفع في اليوم الاسود – على قد لحافك مد رجل

  1. 10.

    Judgment

  • Participants were asked

“If you find a stamped, addressed envelope lying on the sidewalk, what would you do?”

  • Answers were scored according to the well-known base in Egyptian culture, e.g., the answer is considered right if the participant answered that he/she would put the stamped envelope in the mailbox or deliver it by him/herself.

Step III: cultural adaptation

When designing the battery, the following modifications were done to some battery tests to suit illiterates and culture:

  1. 1.

    Word recognition test

  • The 18 novel words were chosen from a list that includes the most frequent 50 words in Arabic language [39].

Words are:

"حصان – دولاب- صورة- درج- ضابط- برواز-كنبة- فلاح- هدية- ورقة- دبوس- فرشة- أم- كشكول- أمير- كرسي- سرير- طرابيزة- راديو- خالة- أب -خروف-مفتاح- تلميذ- قطة- طفل- عامل"

  1. 2.

    Bender Gestalt copy and memory

  • This figure was chosen as it is known to be free from cultural influence [40] and as it is more simplified to illiterates.

    1. 3.

      Ain Shams Set Shifting for Illiterates (ASTI)

  • Participants were asked to shift between triangles and the corresponding number of circles starting from small numbers to bigger ones.

  • Numbers and letters were avoided to suit illiterates.

  • No colors were used to avoid Stroop effect.

  • Time and number of correct lines were scored.

    1. 4.

      Language object naming test

Three items were changed to suit illiterate and low-educated Egyptian population:

Violin, oud, and crocodile were changed to pot, glasses, and scissors.

Step IV: pilot study

  • Battery tests were first conducted on 15 participants.

  • The participants were mixed between educated and illiterates to make sure that the tests are well understood, could be applied on different levels of education, to evaluate feasibility of the battery, and to adjust study design before applying it on other participants.

Step V: applying battery tests

Battery tests were applied on participants along with MMSE and DSM IV criteria for dementia to validate the battery and measure its ability to differentiate between normal, MCI, and dementia patients according to 3 quadrants of MMSE (scores were divided into normal, MCI and dementia).

Step VI: psychometrics

  1. (a)

    Validity

Criterion group validity was done to compare battery tests of the current study with DSM-IV dementia and MMSE as they are already valid.

  1. (b)

    Reliability

Test re-test reliability was done for 20 participants by the same clinician with 28 days separating the two tests.

Statistical analysis

The statistical analysis was carried out with the Statistical Package for the Social Sciences for Windows version 16.0 (SPSS Inc., Chicago, IL, USA.).

Sensitivity, specificity, positive and negative predictive values, and their 95% confidence interval levels for cutoff points were calculated for MMSE. ROC curve was conducted to determine MMSE cut off points.

Descriptive statistics (mean and standard deviation) were calculated for battery tests. F value and post hoc tests were done to compare variations inside groups.

Results

Demographics

One hundred seventy community dwelling elderly; 78 males and 92 females were recruited, both cognitively intact and impaired; including 64 illiterate and low educated (≤ 9 years education) and 106 with higher education (> 9 years education). Eleven were dropped in application of DSM IV criteria for dementia, so defining cut off points of MMSE was based on 159 participants: 58 illiterate and low educated (≤ 9 years education) and 101 with higher education (> 9 years education).

Some participants had clinical diagnosis; ten vascular cognitive impairment, five traumatic brain injuries, three Alzheimer’s disease, one behavioral frontotemporal dementia, one posterior cortical atrophy, three space occupying lesion, two carbon monoxide poisoning, and three post arrest.

Table 1 showed that test re-test reliability ranged from adequate to high in most tests except distractor interval easy, visuospatial copy and recall, digit span forward and backward, and spherical and letter fluencies. Table 2 compared significance between each battery test in normal and dementia patients diagnosed according to DSM IV criteria for dementia. There was statistical significance between normal and dementia patients in all battery tests except digit span forward length, digit span backward length, stimulus cue of confrontation naming, and judgment. Table 3 presented means and standard deviations of each test according to three quadrants of MMSE in illiterate and low-educated group. High significance between the 3 groups was found for semantic fluency, VL hard total score& 2nd trial, BG recall, CN total score, and with stimulus cue. Table 4 presented means and standard deviations of each test according to three quadrants of MMSE in the group with ≥ 9 years education. There is high significant difference between the 3 groups for all domains except digit span forwards and backwards, confrontation naming phonemic cues, and abstraction. No significance for judgment was found between groups. Table 5 presented correlation between education and battery tests. There was no statistical significance between both groups regarding education except in lexical fluency and backward digit span which are both influenced by education.

Table 1 Test-retest reliability (N = 20).
Table 2 Significant T test for independent groups according to DSMIV dementia (N = 159)
Table 3 Significant F test for between groups accordingly to three quadrants of MMSE within illiterates and low educated (N = 64)
Table 4 Significant F test for between groups accordingly to three quadrants of MMSE within educated (N = 106)
Table 5 Significant F test between Groups as regards education

Discussion

The objective of this study was to introduce a new battery for cognitive assessment in Egyptian elderly and to present the rationale behind its construction. This new neuropsychological battery was designed to overcome defects in existing dementia screening instruments and their inability to detect early cognitive impairment by assessments of verbal learning, short- and long-term memory, cued memory, short-term and working memory, visuospatial functions, executive functions, verbal fluency, and confrontation naming, assessing the effect of depression on those tests and to apply tests that are less affected by education.

MMSE is one of the most widely used tests for cognitive screening all over the world and though conflicts exist regarding its accuracy, the American Academy of Neurology in its guidance and the Alzheimer’s Association in their guidance suggested MMSE as an important tool for cognitive screening [41], so it was used in this study as a standard test using the cut off points ≤ 22 for participants > 9 years education and ≤ 21 for illiterate participants and those with < 9 years education [30].

For the validity and reliability of the new battery, test re-test reliability of each battery subset was done. Significance between battery tests and DSMIV dementia was calculated, then the sample was divided into two groups; participants who are illiterates till ≤ 9 years education and another one with participants > 9 years education. Each group was divided into normal, mild cognitive impairment (MCI), and dementia according to MMSE scores.

MMSE was divided into three quadrants to measure normal, MCI, and dementia and to compare battery tests to it and mean and standard deviation for each battery subset in each group was determined. Then F value and post HOC were calculated to measure significance and to confirm where the differences occurred between groups using criterion group validity.

Following the measures taken by Morrison and colleges in 2015 [42] to test reliability, 20 participants were included in the pre-post sample to test reliability of the current study. The 20 participants took the battery and were invited to take the battery 28 days after the first administration by the same clinician. Test-retest reliability of the current battery was calculated with Pearson correlations and 95% CIs and Spearman’s rho for each subtest.

The analytic test-retest reliabilities of the current battery scores ranged from adequate to high (i.e., r = .7 and higher) in most of the battery subsets. The values of some tests (distractor interval easy, visuospatial copy and recall, digit span length, spherical and letter fluencies) were lower (r < 0.7). These results match the validation results of Calamia and colleges in 2013 [43] which revealed reliability in most of their results except for some memory and executive functioning scores which in both studies could be contributed to practice effects.

Calamia and collegues in 2012 [44] reported in their meta-analysis that practice effects tend to be more evident on certain tests (e.g., memory, processing speed, which can affect recall, spherical, and letter fluencies respectively) than others (e.g., visuospatial abilities, verbal knowledge), with considerable variability within different cognitive domains and some variation with different methods of assessment. They also reported that age, retest interval whether long or short could influence the amount and direction of practice effects.

For validation purpose of the current study, significance between battery tests in normal and dementia patients diagnosed according to and DSM-IV criteria for dementia were measured; results showed significant difference in most of the battery tests; verbal learning, distractor interval, delayed recall, word recognition, figure copying and recall, modified trails, confrontation naming, and abstraction. However, there was no significance between normal and dementia patients regarding digit span, stimulus cueing of confrontation naming, and judgment (judgment almost reached a statistical significance).

Upon classifying the battery tests according to three quadrants of MMSE into normal, MCI, and dementia, among the group with > 9 years education, most battery tests showed highly significant difference between 3 subgroups (e.g., verbal learning, word recognition, figure copy and recall, delayed recall, distractor interval, set shifting time, confrontation naming, digit span backward, lexical and semantic fluency) with less extent of significance for digit span forward, phonemic cueing of confrontation naming, and abstraction.

In illiterate and low-educated people, there was statistically significant difference between the 3 groups regarding semantic fluency, verbal learning, figure copy and recall, distractor interval, set shifting time, and confrontation naming. However, digit span forward and backward, phonemic cuing of confrontation naming, abstraction, and judgment showed no difference.

Overall, the battery showed good ability to differentiate between normal, MCI, and dementia people and the study showed that the battery is valid and reliable to diagnose MCI and dementia.

Regarding education, the current study showed no statistically significant association between levels of education and the following tests; verbal learning, distractor interval, delayed recall, word recognition, visuospatial figure copying and recall, set shifting abilities, forward digit span, animals and spherical fluencies, confrontation naming, stimulus and phonemic cuing, abstraction, and judgment. This denotes the ability of the battery to be applied on illiterate subjects. However, there was highly statistically significant difference regarding backward digit span and letter fluency.

These results are concordant with Kwon et al.’s [45] results which showed insignificance in forward digit span, verbal word recognition, and animals’ fluency regarding education. However, their study stated that the performance of the illiterate subjects was worse than that of literate subjects in visuospatial functions and verbal learning test.

The difference between their study and the current study in visuospatial functions could be attributed to the more complexity of Rey-Osterrrieth figure used in their study in comparison to the simple Bender copy in the current study and to the more time delay (20 min) in comparison to 10 min in the current study. Kwon et al. 2015 [45] applied the Seoul Verbal Learning Test (SVLT) which is formed of three learning-free recall trials of 12 words which is more difficult than the paired associate of Wechsler applied in the current study which compresses three learning associate recall trials of 10 words.

A study done in Turkey by Akdag and colleagues in 2013 [46] using Hodkinson Abbreviated Mental Test (which assessed working, short and long-term memory, attention, orientation, and general fund of information) to screen for dementia in elderly people also found no effect of education on cognitive function.

The new battery of current study will be under the name of Ain Shams Cognitive Assessment (ASCA) scale.

Conclusions

New valid and reliable neurocognitive diagnostic evaluation battery that can differentiate between normal, MCI, and dementia in both educated and illiterate subjects in Egyptian population under the name of Ain Shams Cognitive Assessment (ASCA) scale is now available.

Limitation of the study

Larger studies are needed for correlation between various types of dementia and test results. Imaging were lacking in most of the participants; so localization of damaged brain areas and correlation with test scores could not be done.

Availability of data and materials

Not applicable.

References

  1. Franchi S, Bianchini F. "On The historical dynamics of cognitive science: a view from the periphery". The Search for a Theory of Cognition: Early Mechanisms and New Ideas. Rodopi. 2011; p. XIV.

  2. Pelletier A, Bernard C, Dilharreguy B, Helmer C, Le Goff M, Chanraud S, Dartigues JF, Allard M, Amieva H, Catheline GW. Patterns of brain atrophy associated with episodic memory and semantic fluency decline in aging. Aging (Albany NY). 2017;9(3):741–52.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  3. Harada CN, Natelson Love MC, Triebel KL. Normal Cognitive Aging. Clin Geriatr Med. 2013;29(4):737–52.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  4. Wisdom NM, Mignogna J, Collins RL. Variability in Wechsler adult intelligence scale-IV subtest performance across age. Arch Clin Neuropsychol. 2012;27:389–97.

  5. Administration on Aging, Department of Health and Human Services. Aging statistics. Last modified May 8, 2013. Available from: http://www.aoa.gov/AoARoot/Aging_Statistics/index.aspx. Accessed 17 May 2013.

  6. Mohs RC, Breitner JC, Silverman JM, Davis KL. Alzheimer’s disease. morbid risk among first-degree relatives approximates 50% by 90 years of age. Arch Gen Psychiatr. 1987;44:405–8.

  7. https://www.alz.co.uk/research/ World Alzheimer Report 2018. Accessed 27 Mar 2019.

  8. Prince M, Bryce R, Albanese E, Wimo A, Ribeiro W, Ferri CP. The global prevalence of dementia: A systematic review and metaanalysis. Alzheimer's Dementia. 2013;9(1):63–75.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  9. Berr C, Wancata J, Ritchie K. Prevalence of dementia in the elderly in Europe. Eur Neuropsychopharmacol. 2005;15(4):463–71.

    Article  CAS  Google Scholar 

  10. Montgomery W, Ueda K, Jorgensen M, Stathis S, Cheng Y, Nakamura T. Epidemiology, associated burden, and current clinical practice for the diagnosis and management of Alzheimer's disease in Japan. Clinicoecon Outcomes Res. 2017;10:13–28.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  11. Kim YJ, Han JW, So YS, Seo JY, Kim KY, Kim KW. Prevalence and trends of dementia in Korea: a systematic review and meta-analysis. J Korean Med Sci. 2014;29(7):903–12.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  12. Phung KTT, Chaaya M, Prince M, Atweh S, El Asmar K, Karam G, Khoury RM, Ghandour L, Ghusn H, Nielsen TR, Waldemar G. Dementia prevalence, care arrangement, and access to care in Lebanon: A pilot study. Alzheimer's & Dementia. 2017;10(1016).

  13. Elshahidi MH, Elhadidi MA, Sharaqi AA, Mostafa A, Elzehry MA. Prevalence of dementia in Egypt: a systematic review. Neuropsychiatric Disease and Treatment. 2017;13:715–20.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  14. Ardila A, Bertolucci P, Braga L. Illiteracy: The neuropsychology of cognition without reading. Arch Clin Neuropsychol. 2010;25(8):689–712.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  15. Noroozian M, Shakiba A, Iran-nejad S. The impact of illiteracy on the assessment of cognition and dementia: a critical issue in the developing countries. International Psychogeriatrics. 2014;12:2051–60.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  16. Julayanont P, Tangwongchai S, Hemrungrojn S, Tunvirachaisakul C, Phanthumchinda K, Hongsawat J, Suwichanarakul P, Thanasirorat S, Nasreddine ZS. The Montreal Cognitive Assessment-Basic: a screening tool for mild cognitive impairment in illiterate and low-educated elderly adults. J Am Geriatr Soc. 2015;63(12):2550–4.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  17. www.capmas.gov.eg/. Accessed Mar 2018.

  18. Phung TK, Chaayaa M, Asmar K, Atweh S, Ghusn H, Khoury RM, Prince M, Waldemar G. Performance of the 16-Item Informant Questionnaire on cognitive decline for the elderly (IQCODE) in an Arabic-speaking older population. Dement Geriatr Cogn Disord. 2015;40(5-6):276–89.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  19. Valkanova V, Ebmeier KP, Allan CL. Depression is linked to dementia in older adults. Practitioner. 2017;261(1800):11–5.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  20. Harvey PD. Clinical applications of neuropsychological assessment. Dialogues Clin Neurosci. 2012;14(1):91–9.

    PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  21. Fasfous AF, Al-Joudi HF, Puente AE, Pérez-García M. Neuropsychological measures in the Arab world: a systematic review. Neuropsychol Rev. 2017;27(2):158–73.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  22. Qassem T, Khater MS, Emara T, Rasheedy D, Tawfik HM, Mohammedin AS, nasTolba MF, Abdel Aziz K. Normative data for healthy adult performance on the Egyptian–Arabic Addenbrooke’s Cognitive Examination III. Middle East Current Psychiatry. 2015;22:27–36.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  23. Kramer JH, Mungas D, Possin KL, Rankin KP, Boxer AL, Rosen HJ, Bostrom A, Sinha L, Berhel A, Widmeyer M. NIH EXAMINER: Conceptualization and development of an executive function battery. J Int Neuropsychol Soc. 2014;20(1):11–9.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  24. Nasreddine ZS, Phillips NA, Bédirian V, Charbonneau S, Whitehead V, Collin I, Cummings JL, Chertkow H. The Montreal cognitive assessment, MoCA: a brief screening tool for mild cognitive impairment. J Am Geriatr Soc. 2005;53(4):695–9.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  25. McKhann GM, Knopman DS, Chertkow H, Hyman BT, Jack CR Jr, Kawas CH, Klunk WE, Koroshetz WJ, Manly JJ, Mayeux R, Mohs RC, Morris JC, Rossor MN, Scheltens P, Carrillo MC, Thies B, Weintraub S, Phelps CH. The diagnosis of dementia due to Alzheimer’s disease: Recommendations from the National Institute on Aging-Alzheimer’s Association workgroups on diagnostic guidelines for Alzheimer’s disease. Alzheimers Dement. 2011;7(3):263–9.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  26. Petersen RC, Smith GE, Waring SC, Ivnik RJ, Tangalos EG, Kokmen E. Mild cognitive impairment: clinical characterization and outcome. Arch Neurol. 1999;56:303–8.

    Article  CAS  Google Scholar 

  27. Folstein MF, Folstein SE, McHugh PR. "Mini-mental state". A practical method for grading the cognitive state of patients for the clinician. J Psychiatr Res. 1975;12(3):189–98.

    Article  CAS  Google Scholar 

  28. American Psychiatric Association. Diagnostic and statistical manual of mental disorders, 4th edition. Washington, D.C: American Psychiatric Press; 1994.

  29. ElKholy N, Tawfik HM, Ebeid S, Hamza SA, Madkor ORE. Defining Cut-off Scores for MMSE in an Educated and Illiterate Arabic Speaking Egyptian Elderly Population. EJGG. 2018;6(1):31–3.

  30. Wang HC, Wass M, Castles A. Paired-associate learning ability accounts for unique variance in orthographic learning. Sci Stud Reading. 2016;21:5–16.

  31. Miller JB, Axelrod BN, Rapport LJ, Hanks RA, Bashem JR, Schutte C. Substitution of California verbal learning test, second edition for verbal paired associates on the Wechsler memory scale, Fourth Edition. Clin Neuropsychol. 2012;26(4):599–608.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  32. Uttl B, Graf P, Richter LK. Verbal paired associates tests limits on validity and reliability. Arch Clin Neuropsychol. 2002;17(6):567–81.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  33. Zhao Q, Lv Y, Zhou Y, Hong Z, Guo Q. Short-term delayed recall of auditory verbal learning test is equivalent to long-term delayed recall for identifying amnestic mild cognitive impairment. PLoS One. 2012;7(12):e51157.

    Article  CAS  Google Scholar 

  34. Keppeke Lde F, Cintra Ide P, Schoen TH. Bender Visual-motor gestalt test in adolescents: relationship between visual-motor development and the Tanner Stages. Perceptual & Motor Skills. 2013;117(1):1299–317.

    Google Scholar 

  35. Kaufman AS and Lichtenberger OE. Assessing adolescent and adult intelligence (3rd ed.). Hoboken: Wiley; 2005. p. 3.

  36. Smagula SF, Karim HT, Lenze EJ, Butters MA, Wu GF, Mulsant BH, Reynolds CF, Aizenstein HJ. Gray matter regions statistically mediating the cross-sectional association of eotaxin and set shifting among older adults with major depressive disorder. Int J Geriatr Psychiatry. 2017; Dec;32(12):1226–32.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  37. Kim HJ, Baek MJ, Kim SY. Alternative type of the trail making test in nonnative english-speakers: the trail making test-black & white. PLoS ONE. 2014;9(2):e89078.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  38. Luhrmann TM. Of Two Minds: An anthropologist looks at American psychiatry. 2011; P 37.

  39. Taha (Iman Abdel Halim) (2004). The effect of post-traumatic stress disorder on the efficiency of some cognitive functions and psychosocial compatibility of a sample of traumatized people. PhD Thesis, Cairo University, Faculty of Arts, Department of Psychology.

  40. Dos Santos AA, Noronha AP, Rueda FJ, Segovia JL. Bender-gradual scoring system: performance of Brazilian and Peruvian children. Percept Mot Skills. 2014;118(3):897–908.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  41. Mitchell AJ. A meta-analysis of the accuracy of the mini-mental state examination in the detection of dementia and mild cognitive impairment. J. Psychiatr. Res. 2009;43:411–31.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  42. Morrison GE, Simone CM, NG NF, Hardy JL. Reliability and validity of the NeuroCognitive Performance Test, a web-based neuropsychological assessment. Front Psychol. 2015;6:1652.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  43. Calamia M, Markon K, Tranel D. The robust reliability of neuropsychological measures: meta-analyses of test-retest correlations. Clin Neuropsychol. 2013;27(7):1077–105.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  44. Calamia M, Markon K, Tranel D. Scoring higher the second time around: Meta-analyses of practice effects in neuropsychological assessment. The Clinical Neuropsychologist. 2012;26(4):543–70.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  45. Kwon OD, Yoon U, Na DL. Effect of illiteracy on cognition and cerebral morphology in later life. Dement Neurocognitive Disord. 2015;14(4):149–57.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  46. Akdag B, Telci EA, Cavlak U. Factors affecting cognitive function in older adults: a Turkish sample. International Journal of Gerontology. 2013;7(3):137–41.

    Article  Google Scholar 

Download references

Acknowledgements

Not applicable.

Funding

No funding in the study.

Author information

Authors and Affiliations

Authors

Contributions

All authors designed the study. EN managed collection of data from study subjects and THM managed analysis of data in preparation for statistical analysis. EN and THM wrote the first draft of the manuscript. All authors contributed to and have approved the final manuscript.

Corresponding author

Correspondence to Heba Mohamed Tawfik.

Ethics declarations

Ethics approval and consent to participate

The study was approved by Faculty of Medicine Ain-Shams University (FMASU) Research Ethics Committee (REC) on 26/6/2016 with reference number 156/2016. The FMASU REC operated according to guidelines of the International Council on Harmonization and the Islamic Organization for Medical Sciences, the United States Office for Human Research Protections and the United States Code of Federal Regulations and operates under Federal Wide Assurance No., FWA 000017585.

Consent for publication

A written consent was obtained from all participants; described below in details.

Competing interests

The authors declare that they have no competing interest.

Additional information

Publisher’s Note

Springer Nature remains neutral with regard to jurisdictional claims in published maps and institutional affiliations.

Rights and permissions

Open Access This article is licensed under a Creative Commons Attribution 4.0 International License, which permits use, sharing, adaptation, distribution and reproduction in any medium or format, as long as you give appropriate credit to the original author(s) and the source, provide a link to the Creative Commons licence, and indicate if changes were made. The images or other third party material in this article are included in the article's Creative Commons licence, unless indicated otherwise in a credit line to the material. If material is not included in the article's Creative Commons licence and your intended use is not permitted by statutory regulation or exceeds the permitted use, you will need to obtain permission directly from the copyright holder. To view a copy of this licence, visit http://creativecommons.org/licenses/by/4.0/.

Reprints and permissions

About this article

Check for updates. Verify currency and authenticity via CrossMark

Cite this article

ElKholy, N., Tawfik, H.M., Ebeid, S. et al. A model of cognitive evaluation battery for diagnosis of mild cognitive impairment and dementia in educated and illiterate Egyptian elderly people. Egypt J Neurol Psychiatry Neurosurg 56, 95 (2020). https://doi.org/10.1186/s41983-020-00223-x

Download citation

  • Received:

  • Accepted:

  • Published:

  • DOI: https://doi.org/10.1186/s41983-020-00223-x

Keywords