Skip to main content

Event-related evoked potential versus clinical tests in assessment of subclinical cognitive impairment in chronic hepatitis C virus

Abstract

Context

Chronic infection by hepatitis C virus causes impairment in neurocognitive function in up to 50% of patients which may not be detected by clinical tests.

Aim

Early detection of neurocognitive impairment in chronic hepatitis C patients and investigating the cognitive function in HCV patient by p300 and clinical test.

Materials and methods

The study included 60 patients with chronic hepatitis C and 30 healthy controls. Participants were subjected to a biochemical, hematological assessment, mini-mental state examination, Montreal Cognitive Assessment, P300, polymerase chain reaction (PCR), and fibroscan made for hepatitis C patients.

Results

The digit span, attention, concentration, and memory were significantly lower in patients than controls. The delayed P300 peak latency and the reduction of its amplitude were significantly evident in patients with liver fibrosis than the controls and patients without fibrosis. These abnormalities were significantly higher with increasing the grade of fibrosis. All patients with cognitive impairment (reduced mini-mental state score) had abnormal P300-evoked responses. P300 could detect neurocognitive impairment in some patients with normal neurocognitive functions by clinical test. P300 had sensitivity (100%), specificity (59.26), positive predictive value (75%), negative predictive value (100%), and accuracy (81.67) in the detection of neurocognitive impairment in HCV patient.

Conclusion

Patients with chronic hepatitis C infection had significant impairment in their cognitive functions. This impairment increases with the increase in grade of hepatic fibrosis. P300 can detect minimal and subclinical impairment of cognitive function at early stages of chronic hepatitis with accuracy (81.67).

Trial registration

PACTR on 19 march 2018 retrospectively. Identification number for the registry is PACTR201804003215168.

Key messages

Impairment of cognitive functions increases with the increase in grade of hepatic fibrosis in HCV patients. P300 can detect minimal and subclinical impairment of cognitive function at early stages of chronic hepatitis.

Introduction

Hepatitis C virus (HCV) infection is a serious global health problem that affects 180 million people worldwide. Hepatitis C virus causes acute and chronic hepatitis which can eventually lead to permanent liver damage and hepatocellular carcinoma [1]. HCV infection is considered a systemic disease. Symptoms of brain dysfunction such as fatigue and depression are frequent in patients with chronic hepatitis C than in chronic hepatitis B, and these symptoms improved after clearing the virus with treatment [2]. HCV is neurovirulent. The exact mechanism by which HCV enters the central nervous system is unknown; HCV may cross the blood–brain barrier via infected monocytes using a Trojan horse mechanism [3]. Neuroimaging studies suggest that HCV-positive individuals show altered structure and function of several neuronal systems, including the frontal neocortex, basal ganglia, and connecting white matter tracts [3]. Neurocognitive disorders have been reported in up to 50% of chronic HCV infected patients [4]. HCV patients had significant deficits in attention, learning, psychomotor speed, and mental flexibility; greater impairment was associated with greater fibrosis stage [5]. Forton et al. [6] compared cognitive function in individuals with HCV viremia and those with only anti-HCV antibodies. He found that HCV-infected patients were impaired on more cognitive tasks than the HCV-cleared group and viremic individuals showed specific impairment in power of concentration and speed of working memory [6]. Cognitive deficits may be found in HCV (hepatitis c virus) patients even prior to the development of cirrhosis and liver dysfunction [2]. P300-evoked response is an objective and independent measure of cerebral information processing to avoid the potential bias of fatigue, latent depression, or impaired self-rating in psychometric assessment [7].

This study aimed at early detection of neurocognitive impairment in chronic hepatitis C patients and investigating the cognitive function in HCV patient by p300 and clinical test.

Subjects and methods

The study included 60 untreated chronic hepatitis C patients in addition to 30 healthy persons matched for age, sex, and educational level. The majority of participants achieved a high level of education (secondary school to university). All subjects did not give a history of cognitive dysfunction. We excluded patients with liver cirrhosis, other causes of chronic liver diseases rather than HCV, psychiatric disorders patients, patients with conditions that limit the fibroscan technique (such as ascites), obese patients with BMI ≥ 28, and patients with renal failure, diabetes, or history of drug abuse in the last 6 months. Fibroscan was done in HCV patients to detect the grade of liver fibrosis. The HCV group was subdivided into three groups according to the grade of liver fibrosis (F0–F1, F2, F3). A written informed consent was taken from patients and control before participation according to the guidelines of 1975 declaration of Helsinki. The patients were randomly selected from the outpatient clinic of the tropical medicine department and internal medicine department Minia University Hospital. The controls were randomly selected from outpatient clinic and relatives of the patients admitted. Both patients and control groups were subjected to the following clinical assessment and laboratory investigations, especially liver and kidney functions, blood sugar, HCV-AB (antibodies), HCV PCR, HIV (human immune deficiency virus), and abdominal ultrasound.

Regarding FibroScan, we used M probe for measuring liver stiffness. The device generates a 50-Hz shear wave. The patients were in a supine position. Transducer probe was placed on the skin, between the rib bones at the level of the right lobe of the liver. The patient was holding breath and the operator pressed the probe button to take the measurements. Ten measurements were taken to detect liver stiffness which was expressed in kilopascals (kPa) [8]. Cut-off values were 7.1 kPa for F ≥ 2, 9.5 kPa for F ≥ 3, and 12.5 kPa for F = 4 [9].

Montreal cognitive assessment scores (MoCA) version A [10] was done to all patients and control to detect the mean of digit span, attention, concentration, memory, orientation, and total scores. Total mini-mental state examination (MMSE) scores were calculated as follows: 25–30 was considered normal, 20–24 indicates mild cognitive impairment, 11–19 indicates moderate cognitive impairment, and a score of 10 or less indicates severe cognitive impairment [11].

Regarding P300, oddball task auditory-evoked potentials (peak of p300 latency) using a Nihon Kohden Neuropack MEB-9204 was done. The electrophysiological activity was recorded from three different positions: Fz (forehead midline), Cz (coronal midline), and Pz (parietal midline). Impedances were maintained between 5 and 10 kΩ. Artifacts were automatically rejected at ± 50 μV. The stimuli were presented through headphones at 90 dispel in both ears. The subjects were seated in a comfortable chair, quiet and properly illuminated room, awake and exposed to a series of standard auditory stimuli and small amounts (15 or 20%) of deviant stimuli. Subjects were asked to detect or count the target stimuli (active task) reporting the total at the end of the session. The test was initiated only when the subject demonstrated a complete understanding of the task. The P300 component was defined as the largest positive-going peak occurring as a reaction for the deviant (rare) stimuli within a specific latency window (250–450 ms). Peak amplitude was measured relative to the prestimulus baseline (100 ms), and peak latency was measured from the time of stimulus onset [12].

All analyses were performed with version 20 of Statistical Package of Social Science (SPSS). Qualitative data were expressed as proportions, while quantitative data were expressed as mean + standard deviation (SD). Unpaired Student’s t test was used to compare data between two groups for normally distributed data, while ANOVA was used to compare data between the four groups. Qualitative data were analyzed by chi-square (χ2) test. Statistical significance was defined as p values less than 0.05.

We calculated sensitivity, specificity, positive predictive value (PPV), negative predictive value (NPV), and accuracy of p300 where sensitivity is the probability that a test result will be positive when the disease is present, specificity is the probability that a test result will be negative when the disease is not present, and positive predictive value is the probability that the disease is present when the test is positive. Negative predictive value will be negative when the disease is not present. Positive predictive value is the probability that the disease is not present when the test is negative. Accuracy is the overall probability that a patient will be correctly classified.

Results

The present study included 60 patients in addition to 30 apparently healthy volunteers who served as a control group their age ranged from 20 to 65 years and 70% of them were males. Table 1 shows the demographic characteristics and liver function of the studied groups. The HCV group was subdivided into three groups according to the stage of fibrosis (F0-F1, F2, and F3). As regards MMSE total scores, all of the F0-F1 group (ten patients) had no cognitive impairment (MMSE score 25–30). Fourteen patients (8 in F2 and 6 in F3) had a mild cognitive impairment (total score 20–24) while 19 patients (7 were F2 and 12 were F3) had a moderate cognitive impairment (MMSE total score 11–19). None of the patients had severe cognitive impairment (MMSE total score < 10) as shown in Table 2. In this study, there was significant impairment in digit span, attention, concentration, and memory between patients and control with more impairment in the F3 group (Table 3). The frequency of delayed P300 peak latency was significantly increased in the three groups of chronic hepatitis than controls (P = 0.029). Meanwhile, the frequency of delay was increased with increasing the stage of fibrosis (Table 4). The mean of the p300 peak latency showed more significant delay in F2 (279 ± 52.2) and F3 (329.3 ± 67.8), than the control (251.2 ± 32.6) [p = 0.0157, p = 0.000 respectively], while F0-F1 (278.1 ± 65.1) showed delay in latency of P300 than control, but it was statistically not significant (p = 0.0919) (Table 5). As regards the p300 amplitude, the F2 and F3 groups had statistically significant decrease than the control with p values (0.045 and 0.0001) respectively while F0-F1 had a reduction in amplitude of P300 which was statistically not significant (Table 6). The reduction of the p300 amplitude was significantly increased with increasing the stage of fibrosis. Table 7 shows that all patients with cognitive impairment (reduced MMSE) had abnormal (delayed) peak p300-evoked response while there was a significant increase in the frequency of delayed p300-evoked response among patients with normal MMSE scores with p value (0.036). P300 had sensitivity (100%), specificity (59.26), positive predictive value (75%), negative predictive value (100%), and accuracy (81.67) in the detection of neurocognitive impairment in HCV patient (Table 8).

Table 1 Demographic characteristics and liver function of the studied groups
Table 2 Distribution of MMSE scores among the studied groups
Table 3 Comparison between patients with HCV and controls as regards MoCA and MMSE
Table 4 Frequency of abnormal P300 peak-evoked potential latency between HCV patients and control groups
Table 5 Comparison of the P300-evoked potential latency between the studied groups
Table 6 Comparison between the studied groups regarding P300 amplitude
Table 7 Comparison of MMSE and peak latency of p300 percentage among the studied groups
Table 8 Sensitivity, Specificity, PPV, NPV, and accuracy of P300 in the detection of neurocognitive impairment in HCV group

Discussion

Cognitive dysfunction in patients with chronic hepatitis C virus (HCV) infection is a distinct form of minimal hepatic encephalopathy (MHE). In fact, the majority of HCV-positive patients, irrespective of the grading of liver fibrosis, display alterations of verbal learning, attention, executive function, and memory when they are evaluated by suitable neuropsychological tests. Similarities between the cognitive dysfunction of HCV patients and MHE of patients with different etiologies are unclear [2, 13, 14]. HCV is neurovirulent that may cross the blood–brain barrier via infected monocytes using a Trojan horse mechanism [3]. Microglial cells and, to a lesser extent, astrocytes harbored HCV-RNA sequences and HCV-specific proteins [15]. HCV replicates in the brain and plays a role in the pathogenesis of neuroinflammation [16] with the increase in the proinflammatory cytokines in the brain. There is also an increase in the choline/creatine and myo-inositol/creatine ratios in the basal ganglia. N-Acetyl aspartate (NNA) and NNA-glutamate are raised in the basal ganglia and prefrontal cortex. These metabolites are raised as a compensatory mechanism to HCV inflammation. When these compensatory mechanisms fail, fatigue and possibly other signs of neurocognitive impairment take place [17,18,19,20]. Neuroimaging studies suggest that HCV-positive individuals show altered structure and function of several neuronal systems, including the frontal neocortex, basal ganglia, and connecting white matter tracts [3].

In the present study, the F0-F1 group had no cognitive impairment (MMSE score 25–30). Fourteen patients had mild cognitive impairment (MMSE total score 20–24), eight of them in the F2 group and six in the F3 group; 19 patients had moderate cognitive impairment (MMSE total score 11–19), seven of them in the F2 group and 12 in the F3 group, and none had severe cognitive impairment. There was also a significant difference in impairment in digit span, attention, concentration, and memory between patients and control which increased with increasing the grade of fibrosis evidenced by MoCA and MMSE. In agreement with these results, Hamdy et al. [21] found that most of his accidentally discovered HCV patients showed mild cognitive impairment, which was higher in dominant hemisphere functions: attention, naming, memory, fluency, abstraction, and orientation. However, their patients were accidentally discovered blood donors and recently diagnosed that explain the mild impairment. Hilsabeck et al., [5] who studied 80 patients, also found that HCV patients had a significant deficits in attention, learning, psychomotor speed, and mental flexibility and reported that greater impairment was associated with greater fibrosis stage. Similarly, Forton et al. [22] who evaluated the cognitive performances of 43 HCV patients found that HCV patients had worse concentration and psychomotor speed. Huckans et al. [23] also found that HCV patients have a lower score in memory, recognition, and attention tests than those without the infection. In a different study which was evaluating the impact of HCV infection on cognitive functions in children with normal liver function tests, Abu Faddan et al. [24] found that HCV-infected patients were impaired in more cognitive tasks than healthy volunteers. The most significant differences found on measures of concentration and information processing speed. In contrast, Cordoba et al. [25] and Soogoor et al. [26] both reported no correlation between HCV and cognition (very young subjects); however, for Cordoba, patients were accidentally discovered. Similarly, Abrantes et al. [27] found no evidence of an association between HCV infection and cognitive impairment. However, the small number of subjects examined in the last study may have affected result interpretation.

In our study, we also detected the cognitive function by p300 method. We found that the frequency of delayed P300 peak latency was increased in the three groups of chronic hepatitis than controls (P = 0.029) and that the delay of P300 peak latency and reduction of P300 amplitude were significantly evident in F2 and F3 groups than control group while F0-F1 showed delay in latency of P300 and reduction in amplitude of P300 than control but they were statistically not significant. The prolonged latency and amplitude reduction of P300-evoked responses were significantly increased with increasing the stage of fibrosis. These results were agreed with that of Kramer [2] who studied 100 patients (25 cirrhotic); he found that HCV individuals had P300 delayed latencies and reduced amplitudes. He also reported that abnormal P300 characteristics were not related to the degree of histological or biochemical activity of hepatitis. In another study [28] involving patients with cirrhotic as well as non-cirrhotic liver disease, P300 latencies have been reported to be similar in non-encephalopathic patients as well as the control, thereby suggesting that P300 latencies were not sensitive in detection of subacute hepatic encephalopathy. Moreover, some investigators, Amido et al. [29] and Senzolo et al. [30], reported that P300 was not useful in detecting cerebral function alterations in cirrhotic patients with no apparent encephalopathy. In this study, the percentage of abnormal p300 peak latency in studied patients, which was significantly greater than those of cognitive impairment (measured by MMSE).In our study, we detected the sensitivity, specificity, PPV, NPV, and accuracy of p300. We found that the p300 is a sensitive and accurate method in detecting early changes in the brain function of patients with chronic hepatitis (sensitivity 100%, specificity 59.26%, positive predictive value 75%, negative predictive value 100% and accuracy 81.67%). Ciećko-Michalska [31] suggested that ERPs are a more sensitive method than psychometric tests in detecting early changes in the brain function of patients with cirrhosis. In partial agreement, Jones et al. [32] had also found in the visual ERP study that the prolongation of P300 latency in cirrhotic patients without overt hepatic encephalopathy (HE) may occur in the absence of abnormalities of a standard psychometric test.

Conclusions

Patients with CHC infection has significant impairment of digit span, attention, concentration and memory and this cognitive impairment increases with severity of hepatic fibrosis. Patients without fibrosis show no cognitive impairment by psychometric tests while showing impairment by event-related potential. P300 can detect minimal and subclinical impairment of cognitive function at early stages of chronic hepatitis (F0) with a sensitivity 100% and accuracy 81.67% even with normal standard psychometric tests. A future study is needed with larger F0 chronic hepatitis patients to confirm this suggestion.

Abbreviations

(χ 2):

Chi-square test

ANOVA:

Analysis of variance

CHC:

Chronic hepatitis c

ERPs:

Event-related potentials square

HCV:

Hepatitis C virus

HE:

Hepatic encephalopathy

HIV:

Human immunodeficiency virus

MHE:

Minimal hepatic encephalopathy

MMSE:

Mini-Mental State Examination

MoCA:

Montreal cognitive assessment scores

PCR:

Polymerase chain reaction

SD:

Standard deviation

References

  1. Ashfaq A, Tariq J, Sidra R, et al. An overview of HCV molecular biology, replication and immune responses. Virol J. 2011;8:161.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  2. Kramer L, Bauer E, Funk G, Hofer H, Jessner W, Steindl-Munda P, Wrba F, Madl C, Gangl A, Ferenci P. Subclinical impairment of brain function in chronic hepatitis C infection. J Hepatol. 2002;37:349–54.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  3. Posada C, Morgan EE, Moore DJ, Woods SP, Letendre SL, Grant I, the HIV Neurobehavioral Research Center Group. Neurocognitive effects of the hepatitis C virus. Curr Hepatitis Rep. 2009;8:158–66.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  4. Ashrafi M, Modabbernia A, Dalir M, et al. Predictors of mental and physical health in non-cirrhotic patients with viral hepatitis: a case control study. J Psychosom Res. 2012;73(3):218–24.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  5. Hilsabeck RC, Hassanein TI, Carlson MD, Ziegler EA, Perry W. Cognitive functioning and psychiatric symptomatology in patients with chronic hepatitis C. J IntNeuropsychol Soc. 2003;9:847–54.

    Google Scholar 

  6. Forton DM, Thomas HC, Murphy CA, Allsop JM, Foster GR, Main J, et al. Hepatitis C and cognitive impairment in a cohort of patients with mild liver disease. Hepatology. 2002;35(2):433–9.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  7. Polich J. Updating P300: an integrative theory of P3a and P3b. ClinNeurophysiol. 2007;118(10):2128–48 PMC 2715154. PMID 17573239.

    Google Scholar 

  8. Kudo M, Shiina T, Moriyasu F, et al. JSUM ultrasoundelastography practice guidelines: liver. J Med Ultrason. 2013;40:325–57.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  9. Al-Ghamdi AS. FibroScan®: a noninvasive test of liver fibrosis assessment. The. Saudi J Gastroenterol. 2007;13(3):147–9.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  10. Nasreddine Z. MD. Version MoCA 12 Novembre 2004 © www.mocatest.org. Accessed 14 June 2017.

  11. Folstein MF, Folstein SE, McHugh PR. Mini-mental state: a practical method for grading the cognitive state of patients for the clinician. J Psychiatr Res. 1975;12:189–98.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  12. Hada M, Porjesz B, Begleiter H, Polich J. Auditory P3a assessment of male alcoholics. Biol Psychiatry. 2000;48(4):276–86.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  13. Kramer L, Hofer H, Bauer E, Funk G, Formann E, Steindl-Munda P, Ferenci P. Relative impact of fatigue and subclinical cognitive brain dysfunction on health-related quality of life in chronic hepatitis C infection. AIDS. 2005;19(Suppl 3):S85–92 [PubMed].

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  14. Forton DM, Taylor-Robinson SD, Thomas HC. Cerebral dysfunction in chronic hepatitis C infection. J Viral Hepat. 2003;10:81–6.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  15. Fletcher NF, Wilson GK, Murray J, Hu K, Lewis A, Reynolds GM, Stamataki Z, Meredith LW, Rowe IA, Luo G. Hepatitis C virus infects the endothelial cells of the blood-brain barrier. Gastroenterology. 2012;142:634–643.e6. https://doi.org/10.1053/j.gastro.2011.11.028 Epub 2011 Dec.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  16. Zahr NM, Mayer D, Rohlfing T, Sullivan EV, Pfefferbaum A. Imaging neuroinflammation? A perspective from MR spectroscopy. Brain Pathol. 2014;24:654–64. https://doi.org/10.1111/bpa.12197.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  17. Forton DM, Hamilton G, Allsop JM, Grover VP, Wesnes K, O’Sullivan C, Thomas HC, Taylor-Robinson SD. Cerebral immune activation in chronic hepatitis C infection: a magnetic resonance spectroscopy study. J Hepatol. 2008;49:316–22 [PMID:18538439. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jhep.2008.03.022%5D%2019.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  18. Bokemeyer M, Ding XQ, Goldbecker A, Raab P, Heeren M, Arvanitis D, Tillmann HL, Lanfermann H, Weissenborn K. Evidence for neuroinflammation and neuroprotection in HCV infection-associated encephalopathy. Gut. 2011;60:370–7. [PMID: 20926642. https://doi.org/10.1136/gut.2010.217976.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  19. Glodzik-Sobanska L, Li J, Mosconi L, Slowik A, Walecki J, Szczudlik A, Sobiecka B, de Leon MJ. Prefrontal N-acetylaspartate and poststroke recovery: a longitudinal proton spectroscopy study. AJNR Am J Neuroradiol. 2007;28:470–4 [PMID: 17353314]21.

    CAS  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  20. Bladowska J, Zimny A, Knysz B, Małyszczak K, Kołtowska A, Szewczyk P, Gąsiorowski J, Furdal M, Sąsiadek MJ. Evaluation of early cerebral metabolic, perfusion and microstructural changes in HCV-positive patients: a pilot study. J Hepatol. 2013;59:651–7. [PMID: 23680314. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jhep.2013.05.008.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  21. Hamdy NA, Rabie SM, Kamal AM, Hasan EM, Helmy AK, Elshayb SF, Neuropsychological HMA. Psychiatric and laboratory findings in accidentally discovered hepatitis C virus patients. Egypt J Neurol, Psychiatry Neurosurg. 2010;47(2):281–8.

    Google Scholar 

  22. Forton DM, Allsop JM, Main J, Foster GR, Thomas HC, Taylor-Robinson SD. Evidence for a cerebral effect of the hepatitis C virus. Lancet. 2001;358(9275):38–9.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  23. Huckans M, Seelye A, Parcel T, Mull L, Woodhouse J, Bjornson D, Fuller BE, Loftis JM, Morasco BJ, Sasaki AW, Storzbach D, Hauser P. The cognitive effects of hepatitis C in the presence and absence of a history of substance use disorder. J IntNeuropsychol Soc. 2009;15(1):69–82.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  24. Abu Faddan NH, Shehata GA, AbdElhafeez HA, Mohamed AO, Hassan HS, Abd El Sameea F. Cognitive function and endogenous cytokine levels in children with chronic hepatitis C. J Viral Hepat. 2015;22(8):665–70.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  25. Córdoba J, Flavià M, Jacas C, Sauleda S, Esteban JI, Vargas V, Esteban R, Guardia J. Quality of life and cognitive function in hepatitis C at different stages of liver disease. J Hepatol. 2003;39(2):231–8.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  26. Soogoor M, Lynn HS, Donfield SM, Gomperts E, Bell TS, Daar ES. Hemophilia growth and development study. Hepatitis C virus infection and neurocognitive function. Neurology. 2006;67(8):1482–5.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  27. Abrantes J, Torres DS, de Mello CE. Patients with hepatitis C infection and normal liver function: an evaluation of cognitive function. Postgrad Med J. 2013;89(1054):433–9.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  28. Davies MG, Rowan MJ, MacMathuna P, Keeling PW, Weir DG, Feely J. The auditory P300 event-related potential: an objective marker of the encephalopathy of chronic liver disease. Hepatology. 1990;12(4 Pt 1):688–94.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  29. Amodio P, Valenti P, Del Piccolo F, Pellegrini A, Schiff S, Angeli P. P300 latency for the diagnosis of minimal hepatic encephalopathy: evidence that spectral eeg analysis and psychometric tests are enough. Dig Liver Dis. 2005;37:861–8.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  30. Senzolo M, Amodio P, D’Aloiso MC, Fagiuoli S, Del Piccolo F, Canova D. Neuropsychological and neurophysiological evaluation in cirrhotic patients with minimal hepatic encephalopathy undergoing liver transplantation. Transplant Proc. 2005;37:1104–7.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  31. Ciećko-Michalska SM, Szewczyk J, Panasiuk A, Słowik A, Wyczesany M, Binder M, Mach T. Event-related cerebral potentials for the diagnosis of subclinical hepatic encephalopathy in patients with liver cirrhosis. Adv Med Sci. 2006;51:273–7.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  32. Jones EA, Giger-Mateeva VI, Reits D, Riemslag FC, Liberov B, Spekrijse H. Visual event-related potentials in cirrhotic patients without overt encephalopathy: the effects of flumazenil. Metab Brain Dis. 2001;16:43–53.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

Download references

Acknowledgements

The authors would like to acknowledge the members of tropical medicine, internal medicine, clinical pathology, and neurology departments of El Minia University Hospital.

Funding

Minia University Hospital helped us in the performance of the investigations to participate.

Availability of data and materials

Please contact the author for data requests.

Author information

Authors and Affiliations

Authors

Contributions

HKFE-B gave the concept of the study and designed the study, recruited the patient, carried out the fibroscan, and participated in the sequence alignment of the study. EA participated in the study design, shared in the recruitment of the patients, carried out the fibroscan together with HKFE-B, participated in the sequence alignment of the study, performed the statistical analysis of the study, and edited the manuscript. WTS carried out P300 and the clinical tests of the study and participated in the editing of the manuscript. DFM participated in carrying out the clinical tests and p300 of the study and shared in performing the statistical analysis. All authors read and approved the final manuscript.

Corresponding author

Correspondence to Elham Ahmed.

Ethics declarations

Ethics approval and consent to participate

The study was approved by Ethical Research Board of Minia School of Medicine, Egypt. Ethics approval date: 30/12/2014. Informed consent was obtained from all patients participated in this study. The study was conducted in accordance with the guidelines of 1975 declaration of Helsinki.

Consent for publication

Not applicable.

Competing interests

The authors declare that they have no competing interests.

Publisher’s Note

Springer Nature remains neutral with regard to jurisdictional claims in published maps and institutional affiliations.

Rights and permissions

Open Access This article is distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution 4.0 International License (http://creativecommons.org/licenses/by/4.0/), which permits unrestricted use, distribution, and reproduction in any medium, provided you give appropriate credit to the original author(s) and the source, provide a link to the Creative Commons license, and indicate if changes were made.

Reprints and permissions

About this article

Check for updates. Verify currency and authenticity via CrossMark

Cite this article

Fath-Elbab, H.K., Ahmed, E., Mansour, D.F. et al. Event-related evoked potential versus clinical tests in assessment of subclinical cognitive impairment in chronic hepatitis C virus. Egypt J Neurol Psychiatry Neurosurg 54, 35 (2018). https://doi.org/10.1186/s41983-018-0034-y

Download citation

  • Received:

  • Accepted:

  • Published:

  • DOI: https://doi.org/10.1186/s41983-018-0034-y

Keywords